Branches of social and cultural anthropology
Contents
Cultural and social anthropology
are distinguishable if not entirely separated intellectual traditions. The use
of the terms "cultural" and "social" to draw the
distinction became common in the 1930s, but the divergence arose earlier, most
directly from the differences between the studies advocated by Franz BOAS (1858
1942) in the United States from the 1890s, and the new directions anthropology
had begun to take in England around that time at the initiative of R. R.
Marrett (1866 1943), C. G. Seligman (1873 1940), W. H. R. RIVERS (1864 1922),
and Alfred Haddon (1855 1940).
Today the two terms do not denote
a precise division of approaches and for this reason some anthropologists have
dispensed with the distinction (e.g., R. Barrett 1984: 2). For many others,
however, the difference remains important, at least as a shorthand way of
characterizing ethnographic styles. The rubric "cultural
anthropology" is generally applied to ethnographic works that are holistic
in spirit, oriented to the ways in which culture affects individual experience,
or aim to provide a rounded view of the knowledge, customs, and institutions of
a people. "Social anthropology" is a term applied to ethnographic
works that attempt to isolate a particular system of social relations such as those that comprise domestic life,
economy, law, politics, or religion
give analytical priority to the organizational bases of social life, and
attend to cultural phenomena as somewhat secondary to the main issues of social
scientific inquiry.
There are several specialized
areas on which over the years anthropologists have focused. Following is a
representation of such different areas which are also popularly known as
branches of social cultural anthropology.
Economic
Anthropology:
Economic Anthropology focuses on two aspects of economics:
(1) provisioning, which is the production and distribution of necessary and
optional goods and services; and (2) the strategy of economizing, often put in
terms of the formalist substantivist debate. Earlier anthropologists devoted almost
all their time to the study of provisioning, but in the last half-century
economizing has received substantially more attention. Formalist-Substantivist
Debate is the dispute in economic
anthropology between those scholars who argue that formal rules of neoclassical
economic theory derived from the study of capitalist market societies can be
used to explain the dynamics of premodern economies ("formalists")
and those who argue that goods and services in the substantive economy are
produced and distributed through specific cultural contexts
("substantivists"). Formalists contend that because all economies
involve the rational pursuit of, access to, and use of, scarce resources by
self-interested, maximizing social actors, formal economic rules can be used to
explain them (H. Schneider 1974). Substantivists, by contrast, contend that
different forms of exchange have different sets of rules and expectations
(Dalton 1961). Following Karl Polanyi the substantivists argue that there are
three major forms of exchange: reciprocity, redistribution, and market exchange
(K. Polanyi et al. 1957). By this view, the rational, maximizing strategizing
that lies at the heart of neoclassical economics and formalist economic
anthropology is characteristic only of market economies.
Therefore, because of the nature
of economic anthropologist’s engagements with substantivist school they tend to
focus on the varieties of ways in which production,
and distribution is done.
Production
refers to the
processes of acquiring resources and transforming them into useful objects and
actions. The objects include food, shelter, and craft goods, as well as
symbolic items ranging from totem poles to pyramids. Before 1940
anthropologists were expected to write a chapter on material culture, which at
least gave us a partial inventory of the objects the culture contained.
Food Production systems are often
categorized into hunting and gathering (or foraging), horticulture,
agriculture, and industry. The underlying dimension of this scale is probably
energy input and energy output: both are low at the foraging end, and both are
high at the industrial end (Leslie White 1943). Given the greater
anthropological expertise with small-scale societies, this scale has more
precision and validity at the low-energy end than at the high end.
Distribution is how goods (and services) are
transferred from one person to another. Most of the effort in economic
anthropology in the last 50 years has been on distribution rather than
production. Very early on it was realized that "primitive" societies
lacked MONEY, or at least our kind of money. How societies could distribute
their goods without money was a key question. Implicated in the answer are
questions of value and questions of property.
The work of economic historian
Karl Polanyi dominated the scene for 30 years (K. Polanyi et al. 1957). Polanyi
proposed that all economies were integrated by one of three major principles of
distribution: Reciprocity, Redistribution, and Market, although the remaining
two were often present in subordinate roles. More recently the dominant scheme,
drawing on MAUSS and Marx, has been GIFT commodity. Reciprocity and
redistribution, and the gift, are forms of distribution that do not require the
use of money.
Issues
of economic anthropology:
PROPERTY rights are implied by the
transfer of goods from one person to another, but are one of the least well
understood aspects of a society and economy. The transfer of a good from one
person to another would seem to require either the concept of property or
force. Yet most anthropological attention has been paid to transfers rather
than the property aspects. Production may also be instrumental in creating
property rights, in that there are natural resources (land, game, clay pits,
etc.), tools, processes, and knowledge that can be, and in larger-scale
economies usually are, the object of property rules.
SOCIAL
STRUCTURE is a
major concomitant of economic organization. Through the process of the division
of labor societies create distinctive units of production, including work
teams, households, men's houses, plantations, firms, and communities. There are
also units of consumption (individuals, households, lineages, and communities).
Property is held by a jural unit, which can be the individual, the household, a
lineage, a village, or a polity. Thus the study of the economics of a society
inevitably requires a clear description of some facets of the social structure.
Population has a curious history
in economic anthropology. Anthropologists have long known that some economic
features are associated with small populations with low population densities
whereas others are associated with large populations and high densities. There
is a strong correlation between population size and the basic form of production
and the distribution of resources. Technology such as domesticated plants and
animals, irrigation, and the wheel permits the emergence of much higher
population levels than are possible in their absence. And some features of
political structure are also correlated with the population size of the
society: political office does not occur in the absence of unequal access to
resources, which in turn does not normally occur in the absence of intensive
agriculture.
This has led to hotly debated
questions over whether technology, social organization, or population is the
driving force in the system. Population has been granted the status of driving
force by such scholars as Boserup (1965), M. Cohen (1977), and HARRIS (1979),
who see a rising population as the stimulus for changing technology, which in
turn permits higher population levels. But if this is the case it does not
explain why some societies, particularly foragers, underproduce and maintain
longterm populations that stay well below carrying capacity (see ORIGINAL
AFFLUENT SOCIETY).
DIFFUSION of culture traits has always
been of interest to anthropologists. One presumes that there has been a
noncoercive diffusion of traits over very large areas (at least continental in
scale) for millennia. Copper, obsidian, and gems are routinely found thousands
of kilometers from their sources at very early times. Some sort of exchange
mechanism is almost certainly implied. Although the spread of physical objects
is easier to document, ideas, tools, and knowledge of processes can spread in
the same way. This model of diffusion based on noncoercive borrowing has more
recently been replaced by the impact of forcible change wrought by CAPITALISM, COLONIALISM, and an accompanying WORLD-SYSTEM that has disrupted small-scale societies at least
since the beginning of European expansion in 1400. As a result diffusion is now
routinely seen as the impact of more powerful societies on less powerful ones,
although a few scholars have objected that a penetration subjugation model of
the process is too simplistic, and that people in the smaller-scale societies
exercise choice and creativity even in the face of powerful forces.
This is brief outline of Economic anthropology (Bilingual, meant for my college students)
Political
Anthropology:
Political
Anthropology devotes itself to the study of law, order, conflict, governance,
and power. Its origins are grounded in concepts drawn from such
nineteenth-century theorists of social evolution as Sir Henry Maine (1861), who
distinguished societies organized by status and by contract in law, and Lewis Henry
Morgan's (1877) distinction between kinship and territory as the basis for the
organization of government. In addition it owes much to the discussions about
the relationships between moral order and social organization found in the
writings of Emile Durkheim (1933), Max Weber (1968), and Karl Marx (1887). More
recent infusions of theory have come from social scientists such as Michel
Foucault (1977b), Pierre Bourdieu (1977), and Anthony Giddens (1984), who focus
on the structure of power in society.
Political
anthropology today is the product of two different legacies. The first, primarily associated with
cultural anthropology in the United States, remained focused on the comparative
and historical questions of how and why political systems evolved. The second, associated with British social
anthropology, was more interested in how politics worked in different societies
and the roles of individuals.
For
decades evolution-minded anthropologists, along with archeologists, have busily
classified societies into categories such as bands, tribes, chiefdoms, and
states, and then debated the merits of one another's typologies (Fried 1967;
Service 1975). Conflict is often accorded a central if not catalytic role in
virtually all these schemas. Yet though war has traditionally been studied as a
means to an evolutionary end (Otterbein 1970), it has only recently been
studied as an institution in and of itself (Turney-High 1949; R. Ferguson 1995;
Otterbein 1994). This new focus on violence in the contemporary world has made
this branch of political anthropology much more salient than in the past. For
example, although the FEUD (as a form of containable conflict) was one of the
first political institutions to be studied, only recently have the
uncontainable effects (and not just the causes) of organized violence in their
various ethnic, political, sectarian, religious, and economic manifestations
become subjects of research by anthropologists (Nordstrom & Martin 1992),
along with possible solutions such as mediation and conflict resolution.
The
second, and perhaps more influential, branch of political anthropology has its
origins in the experience of anthropological fieldwork and the very practical
concerns associated with locating order in non-Western societies. This was the
explicit aim of the founding work in the field, African political systems
(Fortes & Evans-Pritchard 1940b). Based on a set of descriptions and
analyses of centralized and decentralized systems of governance in Africa,
societies were divided into two types: "primitive states" that
possessed government institutions and "stateless societies" that did
not. This work, and examples of detailed fieldwork on political systems such as
Evans-Pritchard's (1940) among the Nuer and Fortes's (1945) among the Tallensi,
inspired a generation of fieldworkers to concentrate on the varied ways in
which political order might be embedded in kinship relations, ritual practices,
age systems, and other order-keeping institutions that did not require separate
institutions of government. Such a focus was of clear concern to colonial
administrators anxious to understand how to govern and control
"subject" peoples, and the role anthropologists may have played in
aiding colonialism has been the subject of considerable debate in recent
decades (Asad 1973; Kuklik 1991). It is clear, however, that the results of
such work, particularly in Africa, pushed anthropology in a number of new
directions.
Later on
Manchester School especially through the work of Max Gluckman have focused on the
central concern of the conflict and conflict resolutions. Political
anthropology, however, was not confined to Africa or the Manchester school.
Edmund Leach (1954) examined the connection between ritual, identity, and
ethnicity among the Kachin of Burma in terms of an oscillating political system
that regularly shifted between ranked and egalitarian forms of social
organization (gumsa and gumlao). Leach's suggestions about the role of
individual agency in politics were followed up by F. G. Bailey (1960) in India
and Fredrik Barth (1959a) among the Swat Pathans to explore the aggregate
effects of political maneuvering.
This is a brief idea of political anthropology (bilingual, meant for my college students)
Psychological
Anthropology:
Psychological Anthropology
approaches the comparative study of human experience, behavior, facts, and artifacts
from a dual sociocultural and psychological most often psychodynamic
perspective. It emerged in the early twentieth century as an attempt to
understand our common humanity, led by such figures as Franz Boas and his
students Edward Sapir, Ruth Benedict, Margaret Mead, Melville Herskovits.
Psychological anthropology displays an arc of theoretical approaches ranging
from scientific positivism, which embraces objectivity and the scientific
method, through various hermeneutic humanisms that emphasize the role of
subjectivity in fieldwork and writing (Suárez-Orozco 1994).
The
origin of such approaches in rooted to Culture and Personality school, which
was a broad and unorganized movement that brought together anthropologists,
psychiatrists, and psychologists who agreed on the mutual relevance of their
disciplines but lacked a common theoretical position, an acknowledged leader,
and an institutional base. Its founders were Margaret Mead, Ruth Benedict, and
Edward SAPIR, all students of Franz Boas, whose influential concept of Culture
had implied a psychological dimension they attempted to spell out and translate
into research. They argued that culture played a role in individual
psychological development (Mead) and in the emotional patterns typical of particular
cultures (Benedict), and also that individuals of a particular society realized
its culture in different ways (Sapir). They criticized psychological theories
that posited Universals for the human species without taking into account human
variability as revealed by anthropological fieldwork in diverse cultures. At
the same time, they were influenced by those psychological and psychiatric
theories that emphasized social influences on the individual, such as the
neo-Freudian formulations of Karen Horney and the interpersonal psychiatry of
Harry Stack Sullivan. Although the movement had no formal organization, its
anthropological founders were joined at seminars, conferences, and in
publications by sociologists, psychologists, and psychoanalysts including
W. I. Thomas, John Dollard, Erik Erikson, Abram Kardiner, Henry A. Murray
and by a growing circle of anthropologists Ralph Linton, A.
Irving Hallowell, Gregory Bateson, Cora Du Bois, Clyde Kluckhohn, and John W.
M. Whiting, to name but a few. The field of culture and personality studies was
very active during the 1930s and in the postwar period 1945 50, as a new
generation of anthropologists conducted studies among Native American peoples
and in the Pacific.
Psychological anthropology has
made one of its enduring tasks to cultivate the theoretical space where the
individual emerges as an active agent in the cultural realm. Historically,
psychological anthropologists have been critical of approaches to the human
condition that privilege one level of analysis (such as the cultural) at the
expense of others (such as the psychological). Sapir, for example, rejected
Alfred Kroeber' (1917a) cultural overdeterminism as espoused in his
"superorganic" model of culture. Sapir (1917) argued that anthropology
could not "escape the ultimate necessity of testing out its analysis of
patterns called 'social' or 'cultural' in terms of individual realities,"
and that "we cannot thoroughly understand the dynamics of culture, of
society, of history, without sooner or later taking account of the actual
relationships of human beings."
This is a brief idea of psychological anthropology (bilingual, meant for my college students)
Cognitive
anthropology:
Cognitive Anthropology is the
study of the relationship between mind and society. Traditionally, cognitive
anthropology examines cultural knowledge in terms of its organization and
application in everyday life, in activities such as classification and making
inferences. Early in its development, in the 1950s, cognitive anthropology was
synonymous with ethnoscience or ethnosemantics. Studies focused on the
structure of conceptual categories in folk classification systems and the
meanings encoded in these systems, in areas such as kinship, ethnobotany, and
color classification. The central unit of analysis was the shared conceptual
category, a conjoined set of distinctive features.
More recent studies of
classification systems have concentrated on the psychological reality of
conceptual categories. If such categories are only conjunctions of features,
then the members of a category should not vary in terms of their psychological
salience. But they do. Take the category "bachelor," defined in
traditional ethnoscience terms as the conjunction of three distinctive
features: male, adult, and unmarried. Men are categorized according to whether
or not they represent the conjunction of these features; there is no middle
ground. This does not represent classification in real life, however, since
popes and priests are not normally viewed as bachelors. It is more likely that
we apply categories like bachelor by matching potential instances to
prototypes, which are stereotypical representations of concepts that serve as
standards for evaluation (Rosch & Lloyd 1978). Since the prototypical
bachelor is promiscuous, non-domestic, and also potentially marriageable,
applying the category to popes and priests would strike most speakers of
English as unnatural.
Research has shown that many
categories are organized around prototypes, from kin terms to furniture (Lakoff
1987; Lakoff & Johnson 1980). Most of this work has been done by linguists,
while cognitive anthropologists have focused on a related knowledge structure,
the "schema," a term borrowed from the early work of F. C. Bartlett
(1932) in social psychology. The difference between a prototype and schema is
that while both are stereotypical, a prototype consists of a specified set of
expectations, while a schema is an organized framework of relations that must
be filled in with concrete detail. Schemas are highly generalized, culturally
specific knowledge structures that help generate appropriate inferences. They
fill in the gaps by supplying information that is usually taken for granted,
thus enabling individuals to identify actions, events, and consequences based
on only brief exposure to only partial information.
Although the evidence that cultural
knowledge structures are highly schematized is strong, studies by cognitive
anthropologists generally avoid making encompassing ethnographic or
psychological statements about whether some knowledge structures are universal,
and (if they are) the extent to which they depend on universal cognitive
processes. For such answers one must turn to the theorists who draw on research
in artificial intelligence and cognitive psychology. They have proposed three
basic models: information-processing models, cognitive-developmental models,
and perception and experiential models.
(1) Information-processing models
attempt to employ important general principles about the architecture of
artificial intelligence systems and their implications for research in human
cognition. Computer models provide a means of assessing the plausibility of
particular proposals. Occasionally, such models have proved sufficiently well
formulated to be subjects in experimental testing with human subjects. At
present there is interest in parallel distributed processing (PDP) and
connectionism, the idea that things that consistently occur together in an
individual's experience become strongly associated in that person's mind
(Bechtel & Abrahamsen 1991).
(2) Cognitive-developmental models
compare cultures to find common developmental traits and themes. Most of this
literature has focused on religious systems and ritual practices. E. Thomas
Lawson and Robert McCauley (1990) borrowed the notion of "competence"
from Chomsky to argue that participants in religious systems represent
knowledge to generate definite intuitions about the "well-formedness"
of religious phenomena. These intuitions are the basis of universal principles
of religious ritual, specifically with regard to the relative centrality of specific
ritual actions. Similarly, Boyer (1994) found that intuitions about religious
phenomena spring from universal principles that act as tacit theories that are
not in themselves intuitive and may require a "leap of faith". For
example, the most widespread ontological assumption of religious systems
postulates the existence of agencies such as SPIRITS whose physical properties
are counterintuitive. Boyer hypothesized that because counterintuitive
assumptions are the focus of more cognitive investment and more emotional
effects than representations of other types, they are more likely to survive
cycles of transmission.
(3) Perception and experiential
models argue that shared perceptual processes and experiences in the
environment shape cognitive forms cross-culturally, a view influenced by
studies of human perception. Most notable in this category are the works of
Lakoff (1987) and M. Johnson (1987), who argued for an "experiential
realism" that is not prey to the conceptual trappings of subjectivism and
objectivism. Lakoff and Johnson began with the premise that the movements of
our bodies and their placement in space generate knowledge structures and modes
of reasoning that are evident in linguistic usage. A central component of their
argument is metaphor. Our thoughts, works, and even our actions are affected by
networks of systematically structured metaphors, which reflect basic kinds of
physiological experience. The difference between up and down, for example, is
an essential aspect of human experience and is evident in English in the
pervasiveness of expressions in which the words "up" and
"down" metaphorically conceptualize moods, states, and emotional
experiences. Other work has focused
Watch this brilliant explanation on how we actually think in terms of categories. by Stanford professor Robert Sapolsky
This is a brief idea of cognitive anthropology (bilingual, meant for my college students)
Legal
Anthropology:
Anthropological studies of law
have been conducted within historical and cross-cultural frameworks and have
contributed to the development of evolutionary, correlational, and ethnographic
theories of social and cultural control. Among eighteenth-century European
intellectuals the idea of Law as universal was common. Nineteenth-century
anthropologists, though armchair speculators, first began to document the
differences between Western and non-Western law. In 1861, Sir Henry Maine
examined materials from Europe and India, arguing that changing relations in law
(from status to contract) were the result of shifts from kinship-based
societies to territorially organized societies. Later researchers, focusing on
dominant modes of subsistence, argued that human societies could be scaled
along a progressive sequence of legal systems from self-help to penal or
compensatory sanctions. For example, Hobhouse (1906) correlated level of
economy with types of law, while Durkheim (1933) associated sanctioning
patterns with degrees of societal integration, repressive law in primitive
societies being progressively replaced by restitutive law in civilized ones.
Malinowski (1926) used direct
ethnographic field observations to question widespread myths about law and
order among preliterate peoples. He called attention to the important
connection between social control and social relations, foreshadowing a
generation of anthropological research on how order could be achieved in
societies lacking central authority, codes, and constables. Radcliffe-Brown
(1933: 202) employed a more jurisprudential approach, making use of Roscoe
Pound's definition of law as "social control through the systematic
application of the force of politically organized society." By defining
law in terms of organized legal sanctions, Radcliffe-Brown concluded that in
some simpler societies there was no law.
Henceforth whether all societies
had law became a hotly debated topic (Pospisil 1958). If law was defined in
terms of politically organized authority, then not all societies had law. If
law was defined as "most processes of social control," then all
societies did have law, but social control became synonymous with law. Today
most anthropologists do not seek to define law either so narrowly or
universally. For purposes of analysis, they increasingly report data in the
categories used by the people studied or in the analytic categories of the
social scientist. Debate continues over whether Western jurisprudence is itself
a folk or an analytic system.
Ecological
Anthropology:
Although the environmental
sciences and environmentalism, including conservation, have roots extending
back many centuries (Glacken 1967), they have crystallized mostly since the
1960s and their use in anthropology has been part of this historical process.
Each of the subfields of anthropology has developed its own approach to human
ecology: paleoecology in archaeology (Butzer 1982); primate ecology (Richard
1985), human adaptability or more narrowly physiological anthropology
(Frisancho 1993), and human behavioral ecology (Eric Smith & Winterhalder
1992) in biological anthropology; cultural ecology and later systems ecology in
cultural anthropology (Ellen 1982; Hardesty 1977; Netting 1986); and
ethnoecology in linguistics (Berlin 1992). Here it must suffice to briefly
review the most important developments in attempts by cultural anthropologists
to understand human ecology and adaptation since the pioneering work on
cultural ecology of Julian Steward and others in the first half of this
century.
Although continuities remain from
the work of Steward (1955) and his cohorts and predecessors, many of the
subsequent ecologically oriented cultural anthropologists have developed new
approaches in response to perceived deficiencies in the work of their
predecessors (Sponsel 1987). Among these are Andrew Vayda and Roy Rappaport
(1968), who developed a systems approach to investigate the interplay of
culture and ecology as human populations adapt to their ecosystem(s). They
systematically applied concepts from biological ecology to human ecology, including
population as the unit of analysis, ecosystem as the context, and adaptation as
the dynamic process of interaction between population and ecosystem. They first
focused on an analysis of energy input and output in the technology and social
organization of work to collect and produce food. All of this was set within
the biological framework of limiting factors and carrying capacity. Components
of culture such as religion and warfare were viewed as regulating mechanisms
that helped to maintain a balance between the population and its resources.
This theoretical framework was elegantly implemented by Rappaport (1967) in his
fieldwork with the Tsembaga of New Guinea. He viewed their ritual and warfare
as regulating the delicate balance between the human and pig populations to
reduce competition between these two species. (Humans and pigs are surprisingly
close in physiology, body and group size, and omnivorous diet). This
"biologization" of the ecological approach in cultural anthropology
led to the label "ecological anthropology" replacing Steward's label
of "cultural ecology," although the two are sometimes used as
synonyms (Bennett 1976, 1993).
Marvin Harris (1979) attempted to
advance the ecological explanation as well as description of cultures by
developing a more explicit and systematic scientific research strategy, which
he called "Cultural Materialism." In this strategy he assigned
research priority and causal primacy to infrastructure over structure and
superstructure because it is most fundamental for human survival and
adaptation. Harris and his students have applied this research strategy to
explain many puzzling customs and institutions. The classic case is the sacred
cow of India. Harris (1985) argued that the cow is not sacred simply because of
Hindu and other religious beliefs, but ultimately because the cow is
indispensable to the agricultural economy in the environments of India,
especially for plowing, fertilizer (dung), cooking fuel (dried dung), and milk
(instead of meat).
The work of Rappaport, Harris,
and others working along similar lines has been criticized on many points, but
especially for confusing origins and functions (Moran 1990) and for assuming
that almost anything that persists is adaptive (Edgerton 1992).
Whereas Harris concentrates on
observable behavior because he is impressed with the discrepancy between what
people say and do, ecologically oriented linguistic anthropologists have
emphasized the investigation of native thought about environmental phenomena.
Much of this work has concentrated on constructing hierarchical classifications
of native terms referring to particular environmental domains such as soil
types for farming or wild plants used for medicinal purposes. Ideally,
ethnoecology encompasses local environmental knowledge, beliefs, values, and
attitudes, and links environmental ideas with actions and their adaptive or
maladaptive consequences. In practice, ethnoecology has often been confined
simply to a native taxonomy of some environmental domain or a mere descriptive
inventory of the names and uses of a subset of animal or plant species (Berlin
1992). However, some ethnoecologists, such as Harold Conklin (1957, 1980), have
gone much further and published unusually detailed data, as exemplified by
Conklin's research integrating the ethnoecology and cultural ecology of the
agroecosystems of the Hanunoo and Ifugao in the Philippines.
Recently, a few anthropologists
have started to transcend some of the limitations of the approaches previously
discussed by adding a diachronic dimension in examining how both culture and
environment mutually influence and change each other over time, an approach
called "historical ecology" (Crumley 1994). Most notable is the work
of William Balée (1994) on the Kaapor in the Amazon of Brazil, who recognize
768 species of plants from seed to reproductive adult stages, the largest
ethnobotanical repertoire reported for any people in the Amazon. Moreover,
Balée has applied historical ecology to integrate aspects of ethnoecology,
cultural ecology, biological ecology, political ecology, and regional ecology
in a processual framework. In this context he has analyzed the Ka'apor's
response to adaptive constraints and opportunities in both their social and
natural environments, including other indigenous societies, Afro-Americans, and
European migrants who have each had an impact on their natural environment.
Urban
Anthropology:
Urban Anthropology examines the
social organization of the city, looking at the kinds of social relationship
and pattern of social life unique to cities and comparing their different
cultural and historical contexts. It emerged as a separate subdiscipline of
sociocultural anthropology during the 1950s and 1960s. In contrast to earlier
studies of urbanism, urban anthropology applied anthropological concepts and
field research methods to urban populations where the city was the context of
the research rather than the phenomenon under study.
This focus is most readily
apparent in the tendency of urban anthropologists to examine the social
organization of small social worlds within the city, analyzing their social
life in terms of larger institutional structures of power. Some of these
studies are based on territorial units such as neighborhoods; others examine
social networks, webs of relationships linking people who may or may not live
nearby. Social networks in cities are frequently nonlocalized, stretching from
rural areas of origin to larger ethnic settlements in the cities (Boissevain
1974; Gmelch & Zenner 1995).
Urban anthropology also examines
social problems characteristic of large cities such as crime, social disorder,
poverty, homelessness, and transience. These studies examine the social
organization and cultural practices of distinct groups within the city such as gangs
(Suttles 1968), ethnic villagers (H. Gans 1962), kinship networks (Stack 1974),
homeless alcoholics (Spradley 1970), and criminals and prostitutes (Merry
1981). These studies usually include the systems of bureaucratic regulation,
urban politics, welfare administration, urban renewal, and economic conditions
that shape local communities. Other research focuses on systems of formal
social control such as police, courts, and prisons.
Despite the concentration of
research on the United States and Great Britain, urban anthropology is a
comparative field. Studies of kinship and neighborhood in British (Michael D.
Young & Willmott 1957) and American cities (Liebow 1967; Lamphere1987) are
paralleled by similar studies in India (Lynch 1969), South Africa (Philip Mayer
1961), Japan (Bestor 1989), and many other parts of the world. Some
anthropologists explore the changing nature of work and union movements in
urban centers in developing countries (Epstein 1958). Others examine the
disproportionate growth of primate cities at the expense of regional towns as a
result of economic development in Third World countries.
Urban anthropologists have worked
extensively on the migration of rural peasants to the cities. This research has
challenged the proposition that as rural migrants settle in cities their social
order and cultural life disintegrates, an argument fundamental to the theory of
urbanism as a way of life. Studies of the squatter settlements that grew up as
a result of rural migrants flooding into the cities of developing countries
during the 1960s and 1970s revealed not anarchy, but emerging forms of social
order, planning, and institutional structure (Peattie 1968; Mangin 1970; B.
Roberts 1978).
Urban anthropology has always
focused particularly on the plight of the urban poor. In his controversial
work, Oscar Lewis (1966) argued that there was a culture of poverty, a uniform
way of life that emerged among the poorest groups in a variety of urban
environments such as Mexico, Puerto Rico, and New York. Although this concept
has been extensively criticized, it was an important effort to theorize the
social impacts of living on the economic fringes of a large industrial city
(Valentine 1968). More recent research views local communities in large
industrial cities as the product of late-capitalist development and the
progressive impoverishment of the poor. Susser (1982), for example, explored
how the changing political economy of the city shapes the life situations of
poor people. D. Harvey (1989b) examined changes in urban life as a result of
global capital and labor flows. Anthropologists examine political and economic
forces that transform urban neighborhoods such as urban renewal,
gentrification, disinvestment in cities, the flight of jobs from the city,
racial discrimination in the private housing market, public housing policies,
and the creation of new towns. Some work explores the way features of
architectural design and urban planning shape social life or foster criminal
behavior (J. Jacobs 1961; Merry 1981). There has been considerably less
published on the way Postmodernity is redefining urban life.
Race, ethnic group, class, and
gender as forms of differentiation and exclusion are fundamental to the field,
and studies frequently examine how categories of race and ethnicity shape
migration and settlement patterns, job opportunities, voluntary organizations,
community institutions, access to work and leisure, and the maintenance of
kinship relationships (Philip Mayer 1961; Mullings 1987). Ethnicity, in particular,
persists in urban areas in the form of ethnic neighborhoods or in such
voluntary associations as rotating credit associations and burial societies
(Hannerz 1980). Thus, urban anthropology, although inspired in its earlier
years by theories of urbanism, now examines social life in the city as it
exists for the people who live in it, rather than the city itself.
Symbolic
Anthropology:
Symbolic Anthropology takes as
its basic tenets the ideas that indigenous meanings are the goal of research
and that these meanings, though not explicit, may be discovered in the
symbolism of such things as myth and ritual. It is a term that marks both an
intellectual movement of the 1970s and 1980s and an anthropological method.
The interpretation of symbolism
per se is nothing new, of course. Presumably, it is as old as literature.
Moreover, it was the stock in trade of the first generation of anthropologists,
in the late nineteenth century. For example, Edward Burnett Tylor (1871) based
a reconstruction of the stages of human mental evolution on what was then known
about "primitive religions," that is any other than a handful of
world religions. By contemporary standards, however, Tylor's interpretations
are naive and ethnocentric. They are naive because, in the rationalist mode of
his time, Tylor began with the assumption that all the complex rites of
primitive religions are a result simply of faulty logic: wrong answers to
questions about real phenomena. Tylor's program is often characterized as
"intellectualist" in a derogatory sense, but J. W. Burrow caught it
better: "the sociology of error" (1966: 7 9). Tylor's interpretations
are ethnocentric because he saw no need to delve into other cultures; on the
contrary,he assumed that he could see directly into the mind of "primitive
man," so that his approach is also called "empathic," again in a
derogatory sense.
In the middle decades of this
century, interest in symbolism retreated as functionalism advanced. The new
paradigm emphasized sociological topics, such as kinship and politics, at the
expense of religion. Moreover, the old symbolic studies were tarnished by their
association with nineteenth century evolutionism, which functionalism
condemned. British functionalists like A. R. Radcliffe-Brown saw themselves as
building a new science of society, and they distrusted, with good reason, the
manner in which Tylor and his contemporaries had dwelt on the bizarre and the
sensational. A concern with symbolism survived only where functionalism failed
to gain the ascendancy, notably in the American culture and personality school,
which included several members with psychiatric training. But their
predisposition toward the universalizing theories of Freud obstructed
culturally specific interpretation and tended to perpetuate the ethnocentrism
of the evolutionists.
In one important respect,
however, functionalism prepared the way for symbolic anthropology by its
insistence on holism. Where nineteenth-century anthropologists had made customs
seem odd by tearing them out of their proper cultural contexts, functionalism
sought to make sense of them by putting them back. It escaped Ethnocentrism by
expecting institutions to be intelligible only as parts of whole social
systems, and symbolic anthropology does the same with regard to ritual and
belief.
Consequently, there was no major
break between functionalism and symbolic anthropology. Moreover, interest in
religion had never completely disappeared; Max Gluckman wrote interestingly
about ritual while remaining the archfunctionalist, for instance in his study
of Swazi royal rites (1954). A re-study by T. O. B. Beidelman (1966)
demonstrates nicely how a symbolic approach might differ from a functionalist
one.
In bridging the two approaches,
the most important figure was Gluckman's student Victor TURNER. Turner began
his research among the Ndembu of northwestern Zambia with issues of SOCIAL
ORGANIZATION, which he found intractable because of the instability of Ndembu
villages. By degrees, he realized that the true continuity of Ndembu life lay
in its rituals and the ideas and values they expressed. To arrive at these
ideas, he devised or adapted methods of interpretation that are best described
in the essay "Symbols in Ndembu ritual," which was first published in
a collection (Gluckman 1964) and later as a chapter in Turner's most widely
read book The forest of symbols (1967). Turner listed three sources of relevant
information: "(1) external form and observable characteristics; (2)
interpretations offered by specialists and laymen: (3) significant contexts
largely worked out by the anthropologist" (1967: 20). In arriving at his
interpretations, Turner moved constantly between these sources, checking one
against another. The key point is that an interpretation arrived at with one
source of data gains conviction when it provides insight elsewhere; it was an
inductive process that genuinely sought Ndembu meanings, and in that lies its
power and appeal.
Philosophical
anthropology:
Philosophical Anthropology is a
branch of philosophy concerned to show that, owing to his preponderantly
underdetermined nature, man is that animal who must, in large part, determine
himself. Although its roots are diffuse and its boundaries fuzzy, in its modern
form philosophical anthropology got its beginnings in the 1920s and was
especially prevalent in German philosophy. It has ties with existentialism,
phenomenology, and Dilthey's "philosophy of life" (in which
consciousness is understood in terms of lived or immediate experience). In its
development it has drawn on a number of outstanding thinkers, including
Kierkegaard, Nietzsche, Pascal, Herder, Goethe, Kant, Hegel, Feuerbach, and von
Humboldt. Recent prominent scholars who may be associated with philosophical
anthropology include Max Scheler, Adolf Portmann, Helmuth Plessner, Arnold
Gehlen, F. J. J. Buytendijk, Medard Boss, Ludwig Binswanger, Erwin Straus, and
Michael Landmann.
What distinguishes philosophical
anthropology is its ontological focus on man as the mediator of his own nature.
According to Herder, in whose ideas philosophical anthropology is rooted, in
man instinct is replaced by freedom; the deficit of specific determinations
becomes a condition for the emergence of reason, understanding, and reflection.
"No longer an infallible machine in the hands of nature, [man] becomes a
purpose unto himself." In effect, a qualita-tive leap is postulated by
philosophical anthropology: " in man something is not simply added to the
animal . . . [rather] he is fundamentally based on a completely different
principle of organization . . . he is the only one who has an open world"
(quoted in Landmann 1982).
Feminist
Anthropology:
Feminist Anthropology takes as
its major premise the idea that the study of women's roles, beliefs, and
practices in society is critical to understanding both the particulars and
potentials of human social life. Although feminist anthropology focuses on women
and women's roles its goal is to provide a more complete understanding of human
society. Most feminist anthropologists believe that the insights gathered in
Western and non-Western contexts should be used to better the lives of people
throughout the world.
Historically, anthropology, like
other academic disciplines, was androcentric with a 'deeply rooted male
orientation" (Reiter 1975b: 12). Bronislaw Malinowski, the founder of the
contemporary anthropological method of participant-observation, typified a
variant of this bias when he quipped that "Anthropology is the study of
man embracing woman" (Moore 1988: 1). But alongside a general demeaning of
the agency of women and the importance of their social roles to the central
meaning of human life, anthropologists were likely to present men's perspective
as the general perspective of a social group.
For example, when anthropologists
wished to study the ritual beliefs of an Australian Aboriginal group they would
study the ritual practices of male Australian Aborigines in the mistaken belief
that they were the most sociologically important practices. In short, men's
roles were not only more centrally studied, but they were also presented as
representative of the entire community's beliefs and experiences. Some of the
earliest works in feminist anthropology countered this presumption by
demonstrating the importance of women and their social and cultural roles to
the anthropological endeavor. They studied women and women's roles in the evolution
of human society, in the maintenance and negotiation of kinship and the family,
and in the operation of global capitalism. Feminist anthropologists posit that
it is only by studying women and men across their various age groups that
anthropology can truly consider itself a student of the cross-cultural variety
of human social experience.
Feminist anthropology and the
broader field of contemporary feminist studies arose in the late 1960s and
early 1970s during what is commonly referred to as the "second wave of
feminism." During these years, Western European and American women in the
feminist liberation movement petitioned for their civil and economic rights.
The academic, social, and political goals of the feminist liberation movement
were developed alongside the black power, native American, and gay and lesbian
liberation movements. All these social groups, with representatives in and outside
the academy, argued that their social perspectives, experiences, and cultural
practices were critical to a proper understanding and appreciation of modern
society. The movements came to be known as "identity politics" and
helped lead to the institutionalization of women's studies programs, ethnic
studies programs, and queer studies programs and to the increase of women and
minorities in various academic disciplines.
The analytic concepts
sex-difference, gender, and sexuality are critical to the methods and theories
utilized in feminist anthropology. The meaning and use of these three terms has
changed over the last one hundred years and are currently undergoing
significant revision. Generally, "sex-difference" is used to refer to
the biological and anatomical differences that exist between males and females.
Thus, whether ultimately referred to genetic, genital, hormonal, brain, or
physiological differences, sex-difference was privileged over sex-similarities
between women and men. This was not always the case even in Western history,
where women and men were once believed to share one sex (Laqueur 1990).
Throughout history, moreover, there have been "hermaphroditic" humans
whose sex organs include components of both female and male physiology.
"Gender" is commonly
used to indicate the meanings and roles that a society assigns to
sex-difference. Gender is what a society makes of the physical, anatomical, and
developmental differences it recognizes. The concepts of masculine and feminine
behavior the type of demeanor,
activities, and speech that "real men" and "real women" are
expected or allowed to have are gender
constructs. They are cultural beliefs that organize social practice, not
biological facts. Indeed, feminist anthropologists have shown that there are no
universal gender roles for women or men. Thus, more than 50 years ago, Margaret
Mead (1935: 16, 18) could speculate that,while every culture has in some way
institutionalized the roles of men and women . . the temperaments which we
regard as native to one sex might be mere variations of human temperament, to
which the members of either or both sexes may, with more or less success in the
case of different individuals, be educated to approximate.
Recently, a number of
poststructural feminists and gender theorists have argued that, in a similar
manner to how it constructs gender, culture constructs sex (Butler 1990). In
other words, all societies, these theorists argue, construct the body
differently, selecting which anatomical differences will be construed as
sex-differences and which will not. Moreover, sex is as available for cultural
manipulation and alteration as gender, especially in technologically advanced
nations. Much of this research has been inspired by, and conceptualized within,
the study of sexuality.
"Sexuality" generally
refers to how a society and individuals within it organize, act on, conceive
of, and represent their erotic and reproductive acts (see sex). Influenced by
the emergence of modern psychoanalysis and psychology, anthropologists have
studied both institutionalized and noninstitutionalized forms of
heterosexuality and homosexuality. Harriet Whitehead (1981: 80) has, however,
noted a parallel between the androcentrism of early anthropology and a
contemporary "anthropological solecism that often appears in studies . . .
interpreting the styles of homosexuality that are fully institutionalized in
the light of those that are not." Irrespective, anthropologists have now
demonstrated that sex, gender, and sexuality are often closely linked concepts
in other cultures and are often utilized for social control. For instance, a
society may attempt to control the sexual practices of some age and gender
groups but not others. Moreover, societies often represent the sexuality of men
and women quite differently: the former as active, virile, and productive, and
the latter as dangerous, polluting, or socially problematic. Societies also
vary widely in how they think about, represent, and regulate the sexual
practices of same-sex couples and cross-sex couples. In the West, same-sex
partners suffer political and economic discrimination. But in many societies
same-sex sexuality is accepted as a vital erotic practice, part of religious
ceremonies, or part of the kinship and alliance systems. Influenced in part by
the works of Michel Foucault, recent theorists of culture and sexuality have
begun to question the applicability of Western notions of homosexuality and
heterosexuality to non-Western cultures.
Visual
Anthropology:
Visual Anthropology is the visual
and perceptual study of culture, material culture, and forms of human behavior
in different communities and environments. As a foundational capability for
doing observational fieldwork, the visual and perceptual capacities have been a
part of anthropological research since the inception of the field. As a
systematized subfield of anthropology, visual anthropology has been undergoing
rapid expansion since the 1960s in terms of theory and practice, as well as the
availability of resources for teaching and carrying out research. Today,
advancing communication technologies are making it possible for anthropological
researchers and film- and videomakers to present elements of their visual and
intellectual experience to a much wider audience worldwide.
Certain basic elements have been
of central concern in visual anthropology since Felix Regnault first shot four
short film sequences of a Wolof woman in Paris in 1895. These include purpose,
depth of knowledge of the subject, nature of the relationship to the subject,
techniques and strategies of expression such as the storyline, themes, editing,
intertitles, narration, voice-over, dialogue, subtitles, style, artistic and
aesthetics sensibilities, and accuracy and film truth (Hockings 1995; Crawford &
Simonsen 1992).
What exactly is an
"ethnographic" film has been a source of anguish to many, and the
debate has not necessarily helped produce more accurate or telling renditions
of social life and behavior. Notwithstanding all the problems of intervention,
interpretation, positionality, and subjectivity, today, with the high-level
word and image interface, film, video, and television are playing a crucial
role in making people better informed cross-culturally. The actual record as images, films, videos is much in demand
by audiences, academic and otherwise, worldwide.
More recently issues of levels
and acknowledgment of collaboration, gender, authorship, indigenous media, and
power have been of concern. Indeed, the concern with power, politics, and poetics
of representation is a serious one for cultural translation, comparison, and
identity. To be behind the camera is to possess technology, power, and
operational knowledge. The dynamics of power sharing and collaboration in the
field and in the sites of production and consumption are complex, and morally
and ethically delicate. To have control and influence over the distribution of
media into today's global market is to have an overriding power to represent
and define the terms of cultural identity and cross-cultural recognition (L.
Taylor 1991; Crawford & Turton 1992; Lutkehaus 1995b).
This is a brief idea of visual anthropology (bilingual, meant for my college students)
Applied
Anthropology:
Applied Anthropology is the use
of anthropology beyond the usual academic disciplinary concerns for research
and teaching to solve practical problems by providing information, creating
policy, or taking direct action. The process takes many forms but is always
shaped by the practical problem at issue, the available disciplinary knowledge,
and the role the anthropologist is expected to play. Since the mid-1970s the
term "practicing anthropologist" has increasingly come to replace
"applied anthropologist" for those in the field. Practicing
anthropologists who apply their knowledge to particular domains (such as
health, development or education) also are increasingly defining themselves
more specifically (i.e., as "medical anthropologist,"
"development anthropologist," or "educational
anthropologist").
The number of practicing
anthropologists has increased significantly in the last 25 years. This trend has
been disguised because the anthropologists often hold titles and play a variety
of significant roles that do not explicitly highlight their disciplinary
background. Such roles currently include those of policy researcher, evaluator,
impact assessor, planner, research analyst, advocate, trainer, culture broker,
program designer, administrator, and therapist, among many others, and the
range is increasing.
Conventional wisdom often assumes
that academic anthropology arose first and that it came to be applied. In fact,
in many areas the relationship was opposite: applied anthropology was often the
starting point of research financed because its sponsors stressed the potential
of its practical benefits. Only later did more exclusively academic projects emerge
and receive support. For example, the first departments of anthropology in
Britain appear to have been touted as kinds of applied anthropology training
programs for colonial administrators. The term "applied anthropology"
itself was, in fact, first used in a 1906 article describing a program for
training administrators at the University of Cambridge. In the United States,
The American Bureau of American Ethnology was established in the nineteenth
century as a matter of national policy. Its voluminous research on native
Americans greatly exceeded that of any university program at the time. This
trend continues today. Much foundational work in legal anthropology, medical
anthropology, urban anthropology, diet, and demography was first done for
applied reasons. Many of the anthropologists who are now regarded as founders
of new fields in academic anthropology were at the time they conducted this
research regarded as not doing "real" anthropology.
Applied anthropology falls into
two general categories: applied research and intervention. Much of the former
is done for reasons of social policy, that is, to help inform the policy
development process either in a specific or general sense, assess the impact of
policy or policy decisions, or evaluate something done because of a policy. It
is often conducted under the rubric of social impact assessment, evaluation,
cultural resource assessment, or technology development research. Intervention
practices most commonly focus on communities rather than individuals. They attempt
to (1) identify community perceptions of need as an important part of the
program-design process and (2) foster the development of empowered community
organizations. Such intervention practices include action anthropology,
research and development anthropology, advocacy anthropology, cultural
brokerage, participatory action research, and social marketing. Although some
anthropologists have served as policymakers, this is still rather rare.
Further readings:
1.
Thomas
Barfield. 1997. Dictionary of Anthropology,
USA: Blackwell.
2.
M.D.
Murphey introductory notes from the website. Type M.D. Murphey Anthropology in
google.
3.
Alan
Bernard and Jonathan Spencer. 2002. Encyclopedia
of Social and Cultural Anthropology. New York: Routledge
4.
This comment has been removed by the author.
ReplyDeleteLocal events
ReplyDeleteWelcome to Cultral.com, Get the cultural stay experience of local events with Cultral.com. We provide opportunities for the cultural experience of local cultural events.
to get more - https://cultral.com/
Sometime few educational blogs become very helpful while getting relevant and new information related to your targeted area. As I found this blog and appreciate the information delivered to my database.bioresonantie haarlem
ReplyDeleteThis blog presented itself in a very easy and clear way. Owner of this blog found a very simple way to express its view but when you read this blog completely, you would get to know about how hard it could be to express such in a easy way.tantra amsterdam
ReplyDeleteThank u for your knowledge
ReplyDeleteThanks for sharing this information. I really like your blog post very much. You have really shared a informative and interesting blog post with people.. dotcom secrets pdf
ReplyDeleteThe post is absolutely great! Lots of great info and inspiration, both of which we all need! Also like to admire the time and effort you put into your website and detailed info you offer! I will bookmark your site!
ReplyDeleteFree Online Courses