Cultural Relativism
Cultural Relativism expresses the idea that the beliefs and practices of others are best understood in the light of the particular cultures in which they are found. The idea is predicated on the degree to which human behavior is held to be culturally determined, a basic tenet of American cultural anthropology. This is often joined with the argument that because all extant cultures are viable adaptations and equally deserving of respect, they should not be subjected to invidious judgments of worth or value by outsiders. Alternatively, some argue that since all norms are specific to the culture in which they were formulated, there can be no universal standards of judgment.
Cultural
relativism in American cultural anthropology is often attributed to the
critique of social evolutionist perspectives by Franz BOAS and his students,
especially Ruth BENEDICT, Margaret MEAD, and Melville HERSKOVITS. Boas
criticized the use of EVOLUTIONARY STAGES as the basis for organizing museum displays,
arguing that exhibits should display artifacts in the context of specific
cultures.
Most
societies are not relativist: they view their own ways as good, other people's
as bad, inferior, or immoral a form of
ETHNOCENTRISM. However, the reverse is also possible, a syndrome Melford Spiro
(1992b: 62 7) termed "inverted ethnocentrism," in which some
anthropologists go well beyond relativism to assert that Western culture is
globally inferior to Primitive or Third World cultures.
Cultural
relativism as an approach can be contrasted with the search for human
UNIVERSALS, the latter often grounded in claims based on such analytic
perspectives as Freudian psychology, marxist political economy, Darwinian
natural selection, or technoenvironmental determinism. Strong cultural
relativists often see anthropology more as an art than a science and prefer to
interpret symbolic meanings rather than explain social mechanisms. Clifford
GEERTZ (1984b) has been an influential spokesman for this approach.
In the
broader philosophical context, cultural relativism is sometimes merged with
cognate forms of relativism (moral, ethical, cognitive, linguistic, historical,
etc.) under the general rubric of Relativism, which is then seen in opposition
to Rationalism, or occasionally, Fundamentalism (see M. Hollis & Lukes
1982). In treating the lively debates on cultural relativism in anthropology
and philosophy, Spiro (1992b) discussed cultural relativism in relation to both
cultural diversity and cultural determinism. Taking the existence of cultural
variation as well documented, as do most anthropologists, he distinguished
three types of cultural relativism
descriptive, normative, and epistemological each with its attendant subtypes.
These
detailed distinctions have not become conventional within the discipline. Most
anthropologists remain content to distinguish the first-order methodological
use of cultural relativism in anthropology from insensitive ethnocentric
attempts to arrive at final ethical, moral, or scientific judgments.
No comments:
Post a Comment